Extraordinary memory skills discovered in a small-brained butterfly
10-21-2024

Extraordinary memory skills discovered in a small-brained butterfly

Deep in the tropics where sunlight filters through the leafy canopy above, the brain of an extraordinary butterfly is puzzling scientists. It’s the Heliconius butterfly, a species that not only indulges in nectar but has a refined taste for pollen too.

This peculiar feeding habit has sparked a series of fascinating studies. The findings were recently published in the journal Current Biology.

Butterfly with a sharp brain

Who would think a butterfly, a creature so tiny and seemingly delicate, would possess cognitive abilities that support complex spatial learning and memory retention?

We’re talking about a butterfly not much bigger than a human thumb, yet capable of remembering where its food sources are and adeptly navigating its way to them.

This ability, researchers believe, is intricately tied to the Heliconius butterfly’s unusually developed brain structure known as the mushroom bodies. This brain structure is responsible for learning and memory.

The butterfly brain

Study lead author Dr. Max Farnworth is a senior research associate in the School of Biological Sciences at the University of Bristol.

“There is huge interest in how bigger brains may support enhanced cognition, behavioral precision or flexibility. But during brain expansion, it’s often difficult to disentangle effects of increases in overall size from changes in internal structure,” explained Dr. Farnworth.

To unravel this complex twist, the research team meticulously examined the changes that unfolded in this butterfly’s neural circuits.

These circuits, much like those found in an electrical system, are composed of cells – the Kenyon cells – that connect in distinct patterns to create a functioning network.

Mosaic brain evolution

The investigation has revealed an unusual growth pattern in these Kenyon cells. Unlike uniform growth in line with the brain’s expansion, these cells have grown at different rates, creating a mosaic of sizes within the brain structure.

This phenomenon, aptly called mosaic brain evolution, paints a vivid picture of a brain’s internal landscape that’s continually changing, with some areas growing while others stay the same.

“We predict that because we see these mosaic patterns of neural changes, these will relate to specific shifts in behavioral performance – in line with the range of learning experiments which show that Heliconius outperform their closest relatives in only very specific contexts, such as long-term visual memory and pattern learning,” said Dr. Farnworth.

Memory and planning skills

To optimize their pollen diet, Heliconius butterflies have evolved a unique foraging pattern. Instead of fluttering aimlessly, they zip along fixed routes between floral resources, much like following a bus route.

Yes – butterflies on a bus route. This unexpected behavior requires planning and memory, so it’s no surprise that the brain circuits underpinning these skills are the hot topic of the study on Heliconius butterflies.

“Rather than having a random route of foraging, these butterflies apparently choose fixed routes between floral resources – akin to a bus route,” said study co-author Dr. Stephen Montgomery.

“The planning and memory processes needed for this behavior are fulfilled by the assemblies of neurons inside the mushroom bodies, hence why we’re fascinated by the internal circuitry throughout.”

Implications of the butterfly brain

The story of the Heliconius butterfly adds intriguing depth to our understanding of evolution and cognitive development in small-brained species.

Insights gained from these butterflies challenge traditional assumptions regarding the correlation between brain size and cognitive capabilities across species.

Despite their small statures, the intricate brain structures of Heliconius showcase immense cognitive prowess, suggesting that evolutionary pressures have profoundly influenced their neural development to optimize for specific ecological challenges.

This raises questions about the adaptive value of these cognitive traits and how they might vary among Heliconius species spread across diverse environments.

Such comparative studies between different species or even colonies of Heliconius can shed light on the evolutionary mechanisms driving brain complexity and its connection to adaptive behaviors.

Future research directions

The discoveries surrounding the Heliconius butterfly’s neurological and behavioral adaptations present an exciting avenue for future research endeavors.

A key area of interest lies in the potential genetic underpinnings that contribute to the unique development of the butterfly’s brain. Further genetic and molecular analyses could provide a comprehensive understanding of how specific genetic expressions are linked to the observed cognitive feats.

Additionally, advances in neuroimaging and electrophysiological techniques open new possibilities for tracing neural activity in real-time, offering a more dynamic understanding of cognitive processing during foraging activities.

These future studies not only promise to unveil the mysteries behind the Heliconius butterfly’s remarkable capabilities but also hold broader implications for the field of neurobiology.

Further investigation of the butterfly brain

With this study adding significantly to our understanding of cognitive innovation in the neural circuits, the research team is set to delve further into the butterfly brain, exploring beyond the memory centers and mapping the brain at an even higher resolution.

“I was really fascinated by the fact that we see such high degrees of conservation in brain anatomy and evolution, but then very prominent but distinct changes,” said Dr. Farnworth.

“This is a really fascinating and beautiful example of a layer of biodiversity we don’t usually see, the diversity of brain and sensory systems, and the ways in which animals are processing and using the information provided by the environment around them,” concluded Dr. Montgomery.

The study is published in the journal Current Biology.

—–

Like what you read? Subscribe to our newsletter for engaging articles, exclusive content, and the latest updates. 

Check us out on EarthSnap, a free app brought to you by Eric Ralls and Earth.com.

—–

News coming your way
The biggest news about our planet delivered to you each day
Subscribe