Dog breed does not define performance and ability 
01-31-2025

Dog breed does not define performance and ability 

For centuries, humans have selectively bred dogs for specific tasks such as herding, hunting, guarding, and scent detection. 

The assumption has been that these breeds developed distinct physical traits, particularly in skull morphology, that make them superior at their assigned tasks. 

However, a new study challenges this long-standing belief, suggesting that skull shape does not determine task performance, and that individual personality and behavior play a larger role than previously thought.

Published in the journal Science Advances, the study was led by Nicholas Hebdon and Lindsay Waldrop.

Misconceptions about dog performance

The researchers used advanced 3D reconstruction techniques to analyze 117 skulls from 40 domestic dog breeds and 18 wild canid species. 

The findings revealed substantial overlap in skull shape across breeds, with no clear structural adaptations for tasks like bite work or scent detection. 

Instead, the research suggests that human breeding preferences have primarily focused on visual traits rather than functional specialization.

Dog skull shape and task performance

The study analyzed dog breeds commonly used for bite work and scent detection, including those in law enforcement and military roles, where dogs are trained for patrol and tracking. 

The researchers compared functional groups as classified by organizations like the American Kennel Club (AKC) and the United Kennel Club (UKC).

Despite the diversity of skull shapes in domestic dogs, the study found high morphological overlap in areas related to function, such as the jaw and nasal structures. This suggests that breeds selected for specific tasks are not as physically specialized as once believed.

For example, bite force measurements showed no significant differences between breeds trained for bite work and those that were not, while scent-detection breeds did not have noticeably enhanced olfactory structures compared to other breeds.

Dogs and specialized tasks

“In the past 200 years, humans have created hundreds of dog breeds that look really different and are pretty specialized at some tasks like herding, protecting, and detecting odors,” said Lindsay Waldrop, an assistant professor of biological sciences at Chapman University.

“We have assumed that these dogs look different because they are structurally specialized at these tasks, but our study shows that, at least for their skulls, they are not specialized for tasks that involve the skull, such as biting tasks and scent work.” 

Myths about “dangerous” breeds

One of the most notable findings refutes a common misconception: that certain dog breeds, such as pit bulls, have unique jaw structures that make them inherently more dangerous.

Waldrop noted that there are many news stories about dogs attacking people badly, and often, specific breeds are targeted in this reporting – such as pit bulls. 

“Some people claim that these dogs bite harder than others of the same size or have special features like ‘locking jaws’ that make them especially dangerous to people. Our study shows that this is simply not true. Dogs bred to bite things aren’t structurally different than dogs bred to do other things,” said Waldrop.

Similarly, no distinct skull adaptations were found in dogs bred for scent detection, suggesting that behavioral traits and training play a more significant role in a dog’s ability to track odors than skull morphology.

Human preferences and dog morphology

The one exception to the pattern was brachycephalic breeds (e.g., bulldogs and pugs), which have shortened snouts due to human-driven aesthetic selection. However, these modifications were not tied to functional advantages.

“I was most surprised by the overall similarity we see in most of the dog skulls,” said lead author Nicholas Hebdon, a postdoctoral research associate at Chapman University.

“Humans have done so much breeding work to alter the visual appearance of these animals that I honestly expected to see really marked groupings of some kind, and we really didn’t see much of that.”

Evolution shapes skull morphology

In contrast, the study found strong differences between domesticated dog skulls and wild canids, such as wolves and foxes. 

Wild canids have longer snouts relative to their cranial length – a feature typical of species that rely on acute senses, such as smell. This suggests that evolution, rather than selective breeding, shapes skull morphology in ways that align with natural survival needs.

Interestingly, fox skulls showed significant overlap with some terrier breeds, which were historically bred for pest control. This structural similarity may reflect functional convergence between wild predators and domestic dogs specialized for hunting small prey.

Breeds and working dog selection

The study challenges the notion that physical traits alone determine working dog ability, emphasizing that behavioral traits and training are more influential.

Recent research has shown that breed-associated behaviors are partially heritable, meaning that some behavioral tendencies – such as herding instincts or scent-tracking abilities – may have genetic components. 

However, the new findings suggest that observable behavior and trainability are more critical factors than skull structure in determining a dog’s suitability for specialized tasks.

This could impact how working dogs are bred and selected, particularly in fields such as law enforcement, search and rescue, and service dog training. 

Rather than selecting dogs based on physical traits alone, trainers may benefit from assessing personality, cognitive ability, and responsiveness to training as primary selection criteria.

New perspective on canine evolution

The study’s findings redefine how we understand dog specialization and selective breeding. While humans have significantly altered the visual appearance of domestic dogs over centuries, functional adaptations may not be as pronounced as previously thought.

By debunking misconceptions about breed-based functional superiority, these findings open the door for more effective, evidence-based approaches to dog training, breeding, and working dog selection. 

Rather than focusing on appearance-based stereotypes, future studies and breeding programs may place greater emphasis on behavioral and cognitive traits – a shift that could benefit both dogs and the people who depend on them.

—–

Like what you read? Subscribe to our newsletter for engaging articles, exclusive content, and the latest updates.

Check us out on EarthSnap, a free app brought to you by Eric Ralls and Earth.com.

—–

News coming your way
The biggest news about our planet delivered to you each day
Subscribe